Brien, 1998; Sanford, 1990; Sanford Garrod, 1998). This was known as resonance, and it can be distinguished from the use of high level representations of events or event structures (that include information about `who does what to whom’) to predictively pre-activate upcoming semantic features or categories (see Kuperberg et al., 2011; Lau et al., 2013; Otten Van Berkum, 2007; Paczynski Kuperberg, 2012 for discussion).Lang Cogn Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.Kuperberg and JaegerPageArguments against predictive pre-activation–By the late 1990s, many psycholinguists were somewhat dubious that predictive PD150606 molecular weight pre-activation played much of a role in normal language comprehension (but see Altmann, 1999; Federmeier Kutas, 1999; Federmeier et al., 2007, and also Tanenhaus et al., 1995, for early discussions of predictive pre-activation in the behavioral and ERP literatures). There was certainly widespread acknowledgment that high level information within the comprehender’s internal representation of context could influence comprehension quickly and incrementally. However, most sentence processing frameworks assumed (either implicitly or explicitly) that such high level information facilitated the processing of new lower level information only after this new lower level information had initially been activated by the bottom-up input. There were several reasons for this skepticism. The first was an intuition that allowing predictive pre-activation to influence processing might afford our prior beliefs too much power, purchase SC144 leading to distortions of perceptual or interpretational reality (e.g. Massaro, 1989). These initial concerns, however, may have been overblown. Within the speech recognition literature, there remain some legitimate concerns that feedback loops between lexical and phonemic representations might lead to auditory hallucinations (see Norris et al., 2000, p. 302 for discussion). However, under the current proposal, lexical inferences based on prior bottom-up input would be used to pre-activate upcoming phonemic information. Moreover, we argue that any predictive pre-activation would primarily influence perception in cases when there is relative uncertainty about the bottom-up input, as in, for example, the phonemic restoration effect (Warren, 1970), or, more generally, processing in the presence of high degrees of environmental noise (McGowan, 2015; Miller, Heise, Lichten, 1951; Stilp Kluender, 2010; Woods, Yund, Herron, Ua Cruadhlaoich, 2010, reviewed by Davis Johnsrude, 2007).10 Similarly, in the sentence processing literature, our prior knowledge, based on real-world knowledge or strongly canonical structures, seems to primarily lead to misinterpretation of the bottom-up input — so-called `good enough processing; (Ferreira, 2003) — when there are strong syntactic expectations (for related discussion, see Kuperberg, 2007). The key point is that these phenomena are, in effect, examples of perceptual hallucinations (in the case of speech perception) or `cognitive’ hallucinations (in the case of `good enough processing’), and the way that they can be explained is precisely through the combination of strong predictive pre-activation and (relative) uncertainty about the bottomup input. A second concern that was sometimes raised about predictive pre-activation is similar to that discussed in section 1: that it may entail costs of inhibiting or suppressing predicted candidates that are not supporte.Brien, 1998; Sanford, 1990; Sanford Garrod, 1998). This was known as resonance, and it can be distinguished from the use of high level representations of events or event structures (that include information about `who does what to whom’) to predictively pre-activate upcoming semantic features or categories (see Kuperberg et al., 2011; Lau et al., 2013; Otten Van Berkum, 2007; Paczynski Kuperberg, 2012 for discussion).Lang Cogn Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 January 01.Kuperberg and JaegerPageArguments against predictive pre-activation–By the late 1990s, many psycholinguists were somewhat dubious that predictive pre-activation played much of a role in normal language comprehension (but see Altmann, 1999; Federmeier Kutas, 1999; Federmeier et al., 2007, and also Tanenhaus et al., 1995, for early discussions of predictive pre-activation in the behavioral and ERP literatures). There was certainly widespread acknowledgment that high level information within the comprehender’s internal representation of context could influence comprehension quickly and incrementally. However, most sentence processing frameworks assumed (either implicitly or explicitly) that such high level information facilitated the processing of new lower level information only after this new lower level information had initially been activated by the bottom-up input. There were several reasons for this skepticism. The first was an intuition that allowing predictive pre-activation to influence processing might afford our prior beliefs too much power, leading to distortions of perceptual or interpretational reality (e.g. Massaro, 1989). These initial concerns, however, may have been overblown. Within the speech recognition literature, there remain some legitimate concerns that feedback loops between lexical and phonemic representations might lead to auditory hallucinations (see Norris et al., 2000, p. 302 for discussion). However, under the current proposal, lexical inferences based on prior bottom-up input would be used to pre-activate upcoming phonemic information. Moreover, we argue that any predictive pre-activation would primarily influence perception in cases when there is relative uncertainty about the bottom-up input, as in, for example, the phonemic restoration effect (Warren, 1970), or, more generally, processing in the presence of high degrees of environmental noise (McGowan, 2015; Miller, Heise, Lichten, 1951; Stilp Kluender, 2010; Woods, Yund, Herron, Ua Cruadhlaoich, 2010, reviewed by Davis Johnsrude, 2007).10 Similarly, in the sentence processing literature, our prior knowledge, based on real-world knowledge or strongly canonical structures, seems to primarily lead to misinterpretation of the bottom-up input — so-called `good enough processing; (Ferreira, 2003) — when there are strong syntactic expectations (for related discussion, see Kuperberg, 2007). The key point is that these phenomena are, in effect, examples of perceptual hallucinations (in the case of speech perception) or `cognitive’ hallucinations (in the case of `good enough processing’), and the way that they can be explained is precisely through the combination of strong predictive pre-activation and (relative) uncertainty about the bottomup input. A second concern that was sometimes raised about predictive pre-activation is similar to that discussed in section 1: that it may entail costs of inhibiting or suppressing predicted candidates that are not supporte.
Related Posts
Regates (Patel et al., 2015). Hence, LLPS seems to become an immense threat element as
Regates (Patel et al., 2015). Hence, LLPS seems to become an immense threat element as the transient localization with the intrinsically disordered proteins into the droplets beneath strain situations, possess the peril of their conformational transitions within the liquid compartments into pathological irreversible aggregates. The phase separation behavior of your RNA binding proteins, appears closely […]
Tive breast cancer cells by modulating expression of aCDase. Such modulation produces two synergic but
Tive breast cancer cells by modulating expression of aCDase. Such modulation produces two synergic but unique events: (1) an increment of Sph-1P levels, which activates proliferative pathways by binding to cell surface receptors and (2) the modulation of cyclin B2 expression, driving mitotic progression and cell development. Another study by Engel et al. [90] showed […]
Ov-Smirnov test. Variance of parametric data was compared working with the “Levine
Ov-Smirnov test. Variance of parametric data was compared using the “Levine’s test for equality of variance.” Comparisons amongst the groups for parametric data had been performed utilizing the Student’s unpaired t-test. Non-parametric frequency data was evaluated for association using a group working with Pearson’s Chi-square test. Non-parametric variables across the groups have been compared making […]